Sabtu, 19 September 2015

!! Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

To obtain this book A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D., you might not be so baffled. This is on the internet book A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. that can be taken its soft file. It is different with the online book A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. where you could order a book and afterwards the vendor will send the printed book for you. This is the place where you could get this A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. by online and also after having handle acquiring, you can download A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. alone.

A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.



A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. How can you alter your mind to be much more open? There lots of sources that can aid you to enhance your ideas. It can be from the other experiences as well as story from some individuals. Schedule A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. is one of the trusted resources to get. You can find numerous publications that we share right here in this website. And also now, we show you one of the very best, the A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D.

If you ally need such a referred A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. book that will provide you worth, get the very best seller from us currently from many popular publishers. If you want to amusing publications, lots of novels, story, jokes, as well as much more fictions compilations are also launched, from best seller to the most recent launched. You might not be confused to take pleasure in all book collections A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. that we will certainly give. It is not regarding the rates. It has to do with just what you require now. This A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D., as one of the very best vendors right here will certainly be one of the appropriate choices to check out.

Finding the ideal A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. book as the best requirement is kind of good lucks to have. To begin your day or to finish your day in the evening, this A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. will certainly appertain enough. You can merely hunt for the tile here as well as you will certainly obtain the book A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. referred. It will certainly not bother you to cut your valuable time to opt for shopping publication in store. In this way, you will additionally invest cash to pay for transport and also other time spent.

By downloading and install the on-line A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. publication here, you will get some benefits not to choose the book shop. Merely link to the net as well as begin to download and install the page web link we share. Currently, your A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D. is ready to delight in reading. This is your time as well as your calmness to obtain all that you really want from this publication A Skeptic's Guide To The Mind: What Neuroscience Can And Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, By Robert Burton M.D.

A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D.

What if our soundest, most reasonable judgments are beyond our control?

Despite 2500 years of contemplation by the world's greatest minds and the more recent phenomenal advances in basic neuroscience, neither neuroscientists nor philosophers have a decent understanding of what the mind is or how it works. The gap between what the brain does and the mind experiences remains uncharted territory. Nevertheless, with powerful new tools such as the fMRI scan, neuroscience has become the de facto mode of explanation of behavior. Neuroscientists tell us why we prefer Coke to Pepsi, and the media trumpets headlines such as "Possible site of free will found in brain." Or: "Bad behavior down to genes, not poor parenting."

Robert Burton believes that while some neuroscience observations are real advances, others are overreaching, unwarranted, wrong-headed, self-serving, or just plain ridiculous, and often with the potential for catastrophic personal and social consequences. In A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind, he brings together clinical observations, practical thought experiments, personal anecdotes, and cutting-edge neuroscience to decipher what neuroscience can tell us – and where it falls woefully short. At the same time, he offers a new vision of how to think about what the mind might be and how it works.
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind is a critical, startling, and expansive journey into the mysteries of the brain and what makes us human.

  • Sales Rank: #520970 in eBooks
  • Published on: 2013-04-23
  • Released on: 2013-04-23
  • Format: Kindle eBook

Review

Praise for A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind:

“Robert Burton's Skeptics Guide provides a thoughtful meditation on the mismeasure of mind. With a rich tapestry of neurological case studies, allusions to film and literature, compelling personal stories, and challenging thought experiments, Burton describes the abundant philosophical and scientific challenges to the belief that we know — or even that we can know — our own minds.” --Daniel Simons, New York Times bestselling co-author of The Invisible Gorilla: How Our Intuitions Deceive Us

"There is no bigger challenge to our self-understanding than the exploding field of neuroscience, but if we are to benefit from its discoveries, we must learn how to think about them in the right way.  And at the moment, we don't.  Thus far, neuroscience research has been oversold by scientists themselves and overhyped by journalists.  We have to do better.  In A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind, Robert Burton does a beautiful job explaining what modern neuroscience has to offer, and just as important, what it doesn't, and probably can't have to offer.  A careful reading of this well-written book will go a long way toward enabling us to draw the right lessons from what neuroscience has to offer."
--Barry Schwartz, author of The Paradox of Choice and Practical Wisdom

“Burton questions the fundamental assumptions of his field – with A Skeptics Guide to the Mind, he takes on the very foundations of cognitive science, leading readers to valuable insights in the process.”
--Vinod Khosla, co-founder of Sun Microsystems

 "This engaging book captures the strengths and limitations of modern neuroscience in unlocking the secrets of mind and brain. It stands alone. In a style that joins academic writing, case histories, and narrative, Burton brings the reader to the many places where person and identity, self and society, health and disease, and, most pointedly, where scientist and social responsibility meet." –Judy Illes, author of the Oxford Handbook of Neuroethics and Professor of Neurology and Canada Research Chair in Neuroethics at the University of British Columbia

“In recent years, there's been a lot of neurotrash infecting everything from economics, business and ethics to romance, gastronomy and parenting. At last, Robert Burton, with the knowledge and wisdom to tackle the subject head-on, dares to separate nonsense from wisdom. With the delicacy of a philosopher and the real life expertise of a physician, he dares to show us how much we've learned but also how much we have to discover. This is one of the most elegant combinations of science and life I've come across for a long time.”--Margaret Heffernan, author of Willful Blindness

“Popular media is awash in an endless deluge of neuroscience findings—particularly those that imply neuroscience is the new arbiter of “truth” for everything from why we like certain colors to whether someone is lying on the witness stand. Readers on the receiving end of neuro-mania are left confused about what to believe, which is why Robert Burton’s A Skeptic’s Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves is such a valuable addition to the library of anyone interested in cognitive science. Burton cuts through the clutter and incisively reveals what the current state of neuroscience is truly capable of telling us about ourselves. It’s a top-tier contribution from one of the leading minds in the field.” --David DiSalvo, author of What Makes Your Brain Happy and Why You Should Do the Opposite  
“Neuroscientific high jinks of the best sort. A salutary reminder that we only understand 10% of our brains.”--Nick Humphrey, author of Soul Dust, The Magic of Consciousness, and Emeritus Professor of Psychology, London School of Economics

About the Author

Robert Burton, MD is a physician, journalist, and author. A graduate of Yale University and University of California at San Francisco medical school, he was formerly chief of the Division of Neurology at Mt. Zion-UCSF Hospital and Associate Chief of the Department of Neurosciences. Burton’s work has appeared in Salon and the San Francisco Chronicle, among others, and he frequently is invited to speak about the brain, the mind, neuroscience, and philosophy of science. The author of On Being Certain: Believing You Are Right Even When You’re Not and three critically acclaimed novels, he lives in Sausalito, California.

Excerpt. © Reprinted by permission. All rights reserved.
1 • The Shape of Your Mind
 
 
It’s unbelievable how much you don’t know about the game you’ve been playing all your life.
—attributed to Branch Rickey1
All complex biological systems—which include you and me—use sensory feedback to monitor their environment. We are made aware of the external world through senses such as sight and sound; we know our interior physical world through internally generated feelings such as hunger and thirst. As the vast majority of thought originates outside of consciousness, it seems reasonable that we would also have evolved a sensory system for informing the conscious mind what cognitive activity is going on subconsciously. Without a method for being aware of this activity, it is hard to imagine what role the conscious mind would have, or even if there would be such a thing as a mind.
If we were cars, our minds would have LED displays telling us what is going on under the hood. But being subtle creatures rather than machines, we have a far more sophisticated system for monitoring subliminal brain activity. Instead of a mental dashboard full of flashing lights, we have evolved an array of cognitive feelings. For simplicity, I’ve used the phrase “cognitive feelings” to refer to those mental phenomena that aren’t normally categorized as emotions or moods, but rather are the type of feeling we associate with thinking. These include such diverse mental states as a sense of knowing, causation, agency, and intention.
To be meaningful, these feelings must bear some relationship to the cognitive activity they are announcing. Just as the feeling of thirst must trigger the desire for drinking fluids, an awareness of a subliminal mental calculation must feel something like a calculation. And here’s the rub. Thirst and hunger are readily accepted as arising from our bodies, but feelings about our involuntarily generated subconscious thoughts often feel like deliberate actions of the conscious mind.
Take an example from the world of visual perception. Imagine yourself at a local football game. You are focused on the game and oblivious of the surrounding faces of spectators. Then, while you are shifting your gaze to look at the scoreboard, your visual system subliminally detects a face in the crowd that it recognizes as your old friend Sam. Your visual cortex compares the incoming image of the face with previously stored memories of Sam’s face and calculates the probability that the face is Sam’s. If the likelihood is high enough, the brain sends the image of the face up into consciousness along with a separate feeling of recognition. You feel as though you consciously assessed the face and determined it was Sam. Depending upon the strength of this feeling of recognition, you will also sense the degree of likelihood that this facial recognition is correct. This might range from a feeling of merely “maybe” or “it could be, but on the other hand…” to a sense of utter certainty.
A visual input of a face, though not initially making any conscious impression, has triggered two separate unconscious brain activities. One is exclusively mechanical and without any associated feeling tone—the comparison of Sam’s face with all other faces previously stored in memory. The other is purely subjective sensation—the feeling of recognition. The two arrive in consciousness as a unit—the visual perception of Sam’s face and the simultaneous feeling that it is indeed Sam. Even though this process occurs outside of awareness, we feel it is the result of the act of conscious recognition. Such lower-level brain activities are experienced at a higher level as voluntary acts.
Because we know the brain is superb at subliminal pattern recognition, we find it relatively easy to concede that recognition doesn’t take place consciously, despite how it might feel. But there are a host of mental sensations that are so intimately linked with our conception of conscious thought that the idea of them not being under our conscious control seems far-fetched.
In my previous book, On Being Certain, I introduced the concept of involuntary mental sensations—spontaneously occurring feelings about our thoughts that are experienced as aspects of conscious thought. Though we feel that they are the result of conscious rumination and represent rational conclusions, they are no more deliberate than feelings of love or anger. My focus was on feelings of knowing, certainty, and conviction—feelings about the quality of our thoughts that range from vague hunches and gut feelings to utter conviction and a profound “aha.”
I now realize that feelings of knowing are a small part of a larger mental sensory system that includes the sense of self, the sense of choice, control over one’s thoughts and actions, feelings of justice and fairness, and even how we determine causation. Collectively, these involuntary sensations make up much of the experience of having a mind. In addition, they profoundly influence how we conceptualize what a mind “is.”
It is vitally important to realize that the cognitive aspect of thought—the calculation—has no feeling tone. Our entire experience of these calculations comes via separate feelings that accompany them into consciousness. For example, though contrary to personal experience, there is no way to objectively determine the origin of a thought. When an idea “occurs to me” or feels as though it “popped into my head,” I tend to label it as arising from the unconscious. On the other hand, if I have the feeling of directly thinking a thought, I am likely to conclude that it is the result of conscious deliberation. The distinction between conscious and unconscious thought is nothing more than our experience of involuntary mental sensations.
This separation between thought (the silent mental calculation) and feelings about thoughts is central to any inquiry into what the mind might be. We know the mind only through our experience; it isn’t something that can be pinned on a specimen board, weighed or measured, poked or prodded. Seeing how our sense of a mind arises from the messy and often hard-to-describe interaction of disparate involuntary sensations is a necessary first step toward any understanding of what the mind can say about itself.
Our Brains, Ourselves
A car cuts you off on the freeway and you get enraged; you honk, flip the driver the finger, fume, and carry on about this brutish lack of manners being a surefire indication of civilization’s impending demise. Your spouse, for the thousandth time, urges you to learn a little self-control. Of course, dear, you halfheartedly agree, your mind oscillating between further thoughts of revenge and the painful recognition that you have just acted like a two-year-old.
You quickly drum up a bevy of seemingly reasonable explanations—a stressful day, a poor night’s sleep, the new anti-hypertensive medication you started a couple weeks ago, long-standing control issues and unresolved childhood slights, your growing apprehension over your declining IRA account. On the other hand, your father had a hair trigger and was prone to seemingly unprovoked tirades and furies. Perhaps you inherited some angry strands of tightly wound DNA. If only there was a straightforward method for self-examination. But your mind reels at the seemingly infinite combination of possibilities, as though the very concept of self-awareness is an overrated myth, a low-probability rubber crutch for the emotionally desperate.
Nevertheless, you have to start somewhere. Though changing your genes is presently out of the question, perhaps you can address your financial concerns. Back at home, you review your IRA portfolio. Your best friend, a financial wizard, gives you a thousand reasons why stock prices are at a generational low and insists that you should “buy, buy, buy.” His arguments are persuasive. You boot up your online broker and poise your finger over the Buy button; but, as though controlled by invisible forces, you have a complete “change of heart” and sell everything. You are puzzled by your behavior. It is as though you have lost control of yourself.
Later that night, flipping through a popular psychology magazine, you read that fMRI studies have shown that the brain region for controlling hand movements is activated before you are aware of making a decision to move your hand. Brain wave (EEG) studies confirm the finding. This can’t be, you think. You try a simple lab experiment. You think about moving your hand but don’t make the final decision to move it; your hand rests quietly in your lap, awaiting instructions. You then consciously decide to wiggle your fingers. You exert some effort, and, not surprisingly, your fingers wiggle on command.
But if the fMRI and EEG studies are correct, your experience of wiggling your fingers voluntarily is nothing more than a comforting illusion foisted on you by an unconscious with its own agenda. Only after the fact did your subconscious let “you” know what it had already decided and acted upon. Looking down at your hand as though it has a mind of its own, you wonder who exactly made the decision. “Who am I?” you ask yourself, at the same time wondering who is doing the asking and who is expected to answer.
To come up with anything remotely resembling a tentative answer to what the mind might be and do, we first need some working understanding of the placeholder for the mind—the self. A mind isn’t an impersonal organ like a liver or a spleen; it is an integral aspect of a self, a part of what makes us an individual as opposed to an object. It’s the center of our being, the main control panel for our thoughts and actions. The self’s central function—creating thoughts and actions—is commonly what we mean when we talk of a mind. Evoluti...

Most helpful customer reviews

29 of 31 people found the following review helpful.
A healthy dose of skepticism
By A reader
The title of the book probably reflects the irony that those who need it most are least likely to pick up this book. This makes the author's effort to call attention to the inherent limit and paradox of neuro- and cognitive-sciences all the more commendable.

I feel this book is more about intellectual integrity than skepticism. The first few chapters explain why neuroscience has its inherent limits with thoughtful discussion and accessible examples. The rest of the book painstakingly picks apart some misconceptions common in popular press. If you are like me, who are interested in, but also overwhelmed by the deluge of new findings in neuroscience, reading this book is a rewarding experience because you are constantly challenged to question your own thoughts and feelings, and in the process you learn more about how to tell information apart from noise. This book probably will not bring one any closer to certainty, but it is a significant step towards wisdom and integrity.

41 of 48 people found the following review helpful.
Easy Read: Informative Perspective
By W Lorraine Watkins
The book leaves for me tantalizing questions; but the overall propositions are solid and bear serious consideration. I hope it will be one of many future scientifically sound challenges to the reductionist neurobiological unitary theories of the mind that today threaten to overwhelm common sense and create much harm in their application.

Dr. Burton writes with a style and language easy for the average reasonably educated reader to follow and understand. There are some flaws but they are really negligible. After first slamming Freudian psychoanalysis, he then proceeds to observe that the only tool we have available for interpretation of all the chemical and anatomic properties recorded with the fMRI and other tools is the individual's conscious mind. Interpretation is defined and understood through individuals' self experience.

He describes a model of the mind that includes levels of consciousness of cognition and their interacting influences on thought. This model of the mind and methods of examination are also perhaps the most important concepts developed by Freudian psychology. Burton also borrows heavily on the work by Piaget and some others without attribution. However these omissions can be forgiven in the service of avoiding conflation with highly charged emotional terms and conundrums.

Another reviewer laments he approaches the difficult dilemmas but avoids deep discussion. I believe this is intentional for the reason that discussing a highly emotionally charged issue would detract from the more important broader perspective and message of the book.

As a psychiatrist and also skeptic of the current rush to the notion of genes and the anatomy of the brain as forming the single door to healing the angst of individuals and societies I am relieved and pleased to see Dr. Burton's caveat to caution.

It is my somewhat informed observation that theories of the mind come in fads to wax and then disappear; as often as not leaving not only a trail of painfully damaged individuals but too often whole ethnic, religious or cultural groups, even large regions of the world in a state of Holocaust. I choose the term "Holocaust" intentionally. This is serious business.

Though Burton spends some time describing the capacity of the unconscious mind to influence interpretation and motivations which really give power to his caveats he fails to suggest the most commonly encountered. In my view, in light of our current times, when many of the most regarded scientists are also holders of personal patents and in terms of compensation highly valued consultants and members of the boards of the corporate sellers of pharmaceuticals; it is negligent to ignore the role of profit motive.

But at last there is a scientifically sound book which represents an important beginning to recognizing the role of emotional cognition, especially at the unconscious level. The second and even more critical observation is of the fact that the conscious, largely unaware mind, is only tool we have to confirm the meaning of the studies of various areas of the brain lighting up on the fMRI. A cabinet maker is only as good as his tools. And what an imperfect tool the conscious mind is!

I am reminded of a quotation of Edith Wharton in her book The Touchstone: "We live in our own souls as in an unmapped region, a few acres of which we have cleared for our habitation; while of the nature of those nearest us we know but the boundaries that march with us."

7 of 9 people found the following review helpful.
A Brave and Timely Book
By NormanMStone@msn.com
The book deserves three stars alone for exposing the multiple problems associated with using the prefix "neuro-" as a syllable to every psychological concept of the day. "Neuro-" this and that is akin to dressing up the emperor in the clothes of science. The multiple problems this produces is presented in a highly readable text that makes the book accessible to the lay reader as well as the professional - the basis for my fourth star.

I cannot give the book five stars because there are significant problems which might have been solved by a more critical editor. The problems are: 1) The very brief conclusion that appears on page 228 appears as a "premise" on page 4! As I understand things a conclusion should follow from a premise - not restate it. As a result the book ends where it began. Thus it could have been better organized both within and between chapters and lacked any narrative tension. 2) The book would have been more appropriately subtitled "What Neuroscience CANNOT tell you". There is very little information on the benefits of fMRI as a tool. In particular in a few places it seems too critical of the use of fMRI (e.g. as a biological marker in the diagnosis of fibromyalgia p> 214-217). 3) The author asserts other authors of papers concerning neurocorrelates of behavior accompany their submissions with an appended biographical statement/apologia to combat the rampant personal biases in this area (!). The present author did not do this and moreover, as previously noted, began his paper with a conclusion marking his work as clearly biased from the start.

See all 21 customer reviews...

A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. PDF
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. EPub
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Doc
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. iBooks
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. rtf
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Mobipocket
A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Kindle

!! Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Doc

!! Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Doc

!! Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Doc
!! Download Ebook A Skeptic's Guide to the Mind: What Neuroscience Can and Cannot Tell Us About Ourselves, by Robert Burton M.D. Doc

Tidak ada komentar:

Posting Komentar